Intuition, Insight, and Degrees of Solution
Our problem solving process is a semi-circular
process embedded within a chronological scale. This includes a repetitive
similarity of process yet not of data analyzed. Each cognitive analytical event
evaluates, compares, reasons, then moves to the next cycle quickly running
through large and small data sets within a much larger set, and with all
hopefully relevant to the problem set to be solved. A recursive process
iterates along with a goal of solution, but of course, not to be confused with circular reasoning. But what is really happening?
Recursive inventory analysis may at some
point tip towards a tentative hypothesis of uniqueness and by insidious
criminal association… individualization. You just can’t separate the uniqueness
from the individualization, nor the crime from the criminal. But how do we
measure the information needed for such a hypothesis? How do we know that we
have this tipping point, this minimally sufficient information that allows us
to understand individualization, a moment of positive recognition in context,
when “No predetermined number of friction ridge features is required to
establish an individualization”? [1] We usually try to articulate some sort of
ratio between quantity/quality of information as compared to the lack of it. An
intuitive ratio? When we reach that tipping point we can typically illustrate
such a feat with a simplified point chart. In most cases it is a level 2 chart
that marks points of interest with red dots. These are points that we, in part,
analyzed. Can it be said that we intuitively know we have reached the minimally
sufficient information that supports a hypothesis of individualization? If the
tipping point is relative to the examiner’s ability to evaluate the problem
set, and the analysis is considered a unique development (non-specificity), it
can’t be otherwise can it? This is fundamentally, degrees of solution. One
examiner will require more or less information to solve the problem. The
important point is that they each successfully surpass the minimally sufficient
relative threshold. Of course, any additional analysis beyond this threshold
would add hypothesis support and lower uncertainty. An analogy is that we don’t
need to compare each and every minutia of a palm print to form a reliable
hypothesis.
We must admit that to understand what
entails a “minimally sufficient” information set, which allows us to recognize
individualization when it exists, is a cumulative compilation of our experience
and our capability applied to that study of that problem. Therefore, it must
also be based, to a significant degree, on intuitive probability. As there is
no sharp well defined threshold, as each examiner will have the potential to
reach a “minimally sufficient” tipping point from their nonlinear analysis
according to their available information and relative ability to correlate and
comprehend that information at that particular point in time. Of course, as the
analytical process progresses one’s knowledge also changes. The old saying of “you
can never step in the same river twice” also applies to our cognitive process.
Our mental database is always changing from moment to moment, and year to year.
We gain new knowledge and forget some as well. What is the probability of my
hypothesis being correct? As this is not computable to any sufficient degree of
accuracy we are leveraged (forced) to rely on our ability to calculate
probability on an intuitive level while we study our rough probability models
in hope that they are at least reasonably accurate.
In a nutshell, the solution, at a
minimally sufficient threshold, has the potential to be discovered holistically
and recursively from the inter-related informational problem set while drawing
from related experience such as training and practice. In the end, a hypothesis
is made of this newly discovered solution that we can illustrate is correct based
on past experience and the concept of uniqueness. How we adjust our applied
knowledge at specific points within the problem solving process is also
critical in our ability to efficiently solve problems. If one particular
approach does not work, then we need to recognize that it may be due to our
process not the lack of information present. An examiner who keeps using a
certain incorrectly acquired target group, may reach a conclusion of No Match.
In fact several examiners may use this same false key, whereas the solution was
to use a different starting point as subtle distortion has had is effects. With
such a scenario it could be said that both the comparison process was
incorrectly applied, and the intuitive probability assessment was based on
false information. It illustrates the potential limits of the human element in
our process. This is an error to be sure, yet is not evidence proving the
concept of individualization is inconsistent. Had the examiner(s) had the
insight to try a new target group, they most likely would have found the
solution.
“Modern research on problem solving has
continued to wrestle with emergent phenomena, such as insight. In the dominant
modern formulation, discontinuous change in problem-solving behavior is assumed
to involve a restructuring of an internal representation of the problem.” [2]
How do we categorize insight, emergence, and intuition into our problem solving
process? Insight can be largely attributed to efficient utilization of our base
knowledge relevant to specific relationships. In effect, a pool of information
and experience we can successfully access and understand in context as needed.
Thus, insight can be the ability to see options. With this outline it is easily
to see how insights can be limited and unrealized.
Emergence can also be described as; “The
rapid appearance of novel structure” within the recursive process. “Examples of
emergent cognitive structure can be trace back to early Gestalt research in
problem solving.” [3] In this light emergence can be thought of as strategic
application and is directly related to insight. While forensic comparison may
not seem to have the need for significant degrees of strategy, the base
comparison search would. That false negative conclusion may have been the
result of poor strategy that failed to produce the needed insight. Or perhaps,
our knowledge base and experience did not contain sufficient information for us
to understand the relative connections, thus solve the problem.
I think it is safe to say that we do
indeed use intuition to understand degrees of uncertainty in our evaluations.
We must, because we can’t run the hard numbers. We are not allowed to run the
numbers. We simply do not have all aspects and variables quantified however, we
may indeed have the information we need to solve the problem itself even if we
don’t realize it.
I see no way to shake the “intuitive
probability” aspect, since we cannot have all the variables inventoried and
well understood. We cannot even make a precise prediction regarding the degree
of uncertainty within any of the relevant variables. Sounds catastrophic
however this is not a bad thing and could even be considered perfectly normal
with this type of problem solving. It is just that most critics want, and in
some cases, demand solid numbers for their comprehension of proof. I consider
this ignorance on their part rather than a weakness on the part of the
examiners. While we will never be able to identify and calculate all the
aspects of uncertainty, we can with proper training and experience, minimize
their negative impacts on our accuracy and get the job done. Uncertainty,
including the application of intuition, does not mean a hypothesis is not
accurate; it simply means we can’t understand and utilize all the information
within a real world problem set. A hypothesis of individualization is based on
a non-algorithmic nonlinear assessment and is made in the face of uncertainty,
albeit with the odds generally on our side. Absolute proof is an eccentric
mathematical concept that seems to exist simply to cause us mere mortals mental
grief. Luckily we don’t have much use for absolute proof. Intuitively, less
than absolute seems to work just fine in the real world.
I like the concept of breaking things
down to better understand their sub-systems and components, yet the best
approach may be a recursive one that studies relationships and insight within a
framework of education and experience coupled with intuitive probability.
Informational sets and processes are revisited and insights applied as new
relationships are better understood in context during our investigation of the
problem. Many processes seem to only work as interrelated groups and by
themselves, may simply become novelties. In our motorsport analogy of a few
posts back, the problem set was to navigate a lap of a race track. This problem
set, not to unfamiliar to any other problem set, is full of relationships and
groups of interrelated actions that surge within a chronological order that
subsequently must work in concert to solve the problem. I would think this is
scalable to the simple problem of comparing two prints. There are many
relationships and groups of inter-related data that when separated out has
little value, but in particular relationships, means everything. A key is to
comprehend the value of the relationships and interrelationships as one works
through the problem. Again, I think this value of these relationships must be
assigned intuitively.
Perhaps specific training in probability
will help. I understand general research has shown that our intuitive
probability skills are underdeveloped.
Craig A. Coppock
Reference:
[1] SWGFAST: STANDARDS FOR EXAMINING
FRICTION RIDGE
IMPRESSIONS AND RESULTING CONCLUSIONS.
100910 draft
[2] The Self-Organization of Insight:
Entropy and Power Laws
in Problem Solving; Damian G. Stephena,b;
James A. Dixona,b,c
From: (Bowden, Jung-Beeman, Fleck, &
Kounios, 2005; Chronicle,
MacGregor, & Ormerod, 2004; Fleck
& Weisberg, 2004; Gilhooly & Murphy, 2005; Knoblich,
Ohlsson,& Raney,2001). Insight
entails an observable discontinuity in a solver’s approach
to a problem indicating a restructuring
of the solver’s representation of the problem;
(Chronicle et al., 2004; Weisberg, 1996)
[3] The Self-Organization of Insight:
Entropy and Power Laws in Problem Solving. Damian G. Stephen/ James A. Dixon
No comments:
Post a Comment